参考文献

Anderson, B. L. (1992). Hysteresis, cooperativity, and depth averaging in dynamic random-dot stereograms. Perception and Psychophysics,51(6), 511–528.

Banks, M. S., Aslin, R. N., & Letson, R. D. (1975). Sensitive period for the development of human binocular vision. Science, 190(4215),675–677.

Biederman, I. (1987) Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review, 94, 115–147.

Birch, E. E., & Stager, D. R. (1985). Monocular acuity and stereopsis in infantile esotropia. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 26(11), 1624–1630.

Bülthoff, H. H., & Edelman, S. (1992). Psychophysical support for a two-dimensional view interpolation theory of object recognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 89, 60–64.

Clark J, Yuille A, (1990) Data Fusion for Sensory Information Processing Systems (Boston, MA:Kluwer).

Doherty, M. J., & Wimmer, M. C. (2005). Children’s understanding of ambiguous figures: Which cognitive developments are necessary to experience reversal? Cognitive Development, 20(3), 407–421.

Drobe, B., & Monot, A. (1997) Partition of perceived space within the fusional area on the basis of apparent fronto-parallel plane criterion. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 17, 340–347.

Edwards, M., Pope, D. R. & Schor, C. M. (1999) Orientation tuning of the transient-stereopsis system. Vision Research, 39(16), 2717–2727.

Farah, M., Wilson, K., Drain, M., & Tanaka, J. (1998). What is ‘‘special” about face perception?  Psychology Review, 105, 482–498.

Farber J., Rosinski R. R. 1978  Geometric transformations of pictured space. Perception 7, 269-282.

Farran, E. K. (2005). Perceptual grouping ability in Williams syndrome: Evidence for deviant patterns of performance. Neuropsychologia, 43(5), 815–822.

Fender, D., & Julesz, B. (1967). Extension of Panum’s fusional area in binocularly stabilized vision. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 57(6), 819–830.

Foley, J. M. (1994). Human luminance pattern-vision mechanisms – masking experiments require a new model. Journal of the Optical Society of America A,11(6), 1710–1719.

Foley, J. M., & Chen, C. C. (1999). Pattern detection in the presence of maskers that differ in spatial phase and temporal offset: Threshold measurements and a model. Vision Research, 39(23), 3855–3872.

Garnham, L., & Sloper, J. J. (2006). Effect of age on adult stereoacuity as measured by different types of stereotest. British Journal of Ophthalmology, 90, 91–95.

Georgeson, M. A., & Georgeson, J. M. (1987). Facilitation and masking of briefly presented gratings - time-course and contrast dependence. Vision Research, 27(3), 369–379.

Gopnik, A., & Rosati, A. (2001). Duck or rabbit? Reversing ambiguous figures and understanding ambiguous representations. Developmental Science, 4(2),175–183.

Green,M. & Odom. J. V. (1986) Correspondence matching in apparent motion: evidence for three-dimensional spatial representation. Science 233, 1427-1429

Hagen M., 1974  Picture perception: Toward a theoretical model. Psychological Bulletin 81, 471-497.

Heisele, B., Ho, P., Poggio, T. (2001). Face recognition with support vector machines: Global versus component-based approach. Proceedings of 8th international conference on computer vision, 2 (pp. 688–694).

Heisele, B., Ho, P., Wu, J., & Poggio, T. (2003). Face recognition: Component-based vs global approaches. Computer Vision and Image Understanding, 91, 6–21.

Heron, G., Charman, W. N., & Schor, C. M. (2001). Age changes in the interactions between the accommodation and vergence systems. Optometry and Vision Science, 78(10), 754–762.

Koriat. A. (1994 ) Object-based apparent motion. Perception & Psychophysics 54, 392- 404.

Kovacs, I., & Feher, A. (1997). Non-fourier information in bandpass noise patterns. Vision Research, 37, 1167–1177.

Kovacs, I., Lukacs, A., Feher, A., Racsmany, M., & Pleh, C. (2001). Contour integration deficit in Williams Syndrome children (abstract). Journal of Vision, 1(3), 146.

Laframboise, S., De Guise, D., & Faubert, J. (2006). Effect of aging on stereoscopic interocular correlation. Optometry and Vision Science, 83, 589–593.

Landy M S, Maloney L T, Johnston E B, Young M J, (1995) Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion. Vision Research 35, 389-412.

Langley, K., Fleet, D. J., & Hibbard, P. B. (1999). Stereopsis from contrast envelopes. Vision Research, 39, 2313–2324.

Legge, G. E., & Foley, J. M. (1980). Contrast masking in human vision. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 70(12), 1458–1471.

Lin, L., & Wilson, H. R. (1995). Stereoscopic integration of Fourier and non-Fourier patterns. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science,36(Suppl. 4), S364.

Maddox, E. E. (1887). Investigations in the relation between convergence and accommodation of the eyes. Journal of Anatomy and Physiology, 21, 21–42.

McKee, S. P., Verghese, P., & Farell, B. (2004). What is the depth of a sinusoidal grating? Journal of Vision, 4(7), 524–538.

McKee, S. P., Verghese, P., & Farell, B. (2005). Stereo sensitivity depends on stereo matching. Journal of Vision, 5(10), 783–792.

Miller, S. M., Liu, G. B., Ngo, T. T., Hooper, G., Riek, S., Carson, R. G., et al. (2000) Interhemispheric switching mediates perceptual rivalry. Current Biology, 10(7), 383–392.

Morgan, M. J., & Castet, E. (1997). The aperture problem in stereopsis. Vision Research, 37, 2737–2744.

Nachmias, J., & Sansbury, R. V. (1974). Grating contrast: Discrimination may be better than detection. Vision Research, 14, 1039–1042.

Naji, J. J. & Freeman, T. C. A. (2004). Perceiving depth order during pursuit eye movement. Vision Research, 44, 3025–3034.

Nawrot, M. (2003). Eye movements provide the extra-retinal signal required for the perception of depth from motion parallax. Vision Research, 43, 1553–1562.

Nawrot, M., & Joyce, L. (2006). The pursuit theory of motion parallax. Vision Research, 46, 4709–4725.

Nguyen, V. A., Freeman, A. W., & Wenderoth, P. (2001). The depth and selectivity of suppression in binocular rivalry. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 348–360.

Norman, H. F., Norman, J. F., & Bilotta, J. (2000). The temporal course of suppression during binocular rivalry. Perception, 29, 831–841.

O’Shea, R. P., & Corballis, P. M. (2001). Binocular rivalry between complex stimuli in split-brain observers. Brain and Mind, 2, 151–160.

O’Shea, R. P., & Corballis, P. M. (2003). Binocular rivalry in split-brain observers. Journal of Vision, 3(10), 610–615.

O’Shea, R. P., & Corballis, P. M. (2005). Visual grouping on binocular rivalry in a splitbrain observer. Vision Research, 45(2), 247–261.

Palomares, M., Ogbonna, C. O., Landau, B., & Egeth, H. (2008). Normal susceptibility to visual illusions in abnormal development: Evidence from Williams Syndrome. Perception, 37, doi:10.1068/p6044.

Pani, J. R., Mervis, C. B., & Robinson, B. F. (1999). Global spatial organization by individuals with Williams Syndrome. Psychological Science, 10(5),453–458

Patel, S. S., Ogmen, H., White, J. M., & Jiang, B. C. (1997). Neural network model of short-term horizontal disparity vergence dynamics. Vision Research, 37, 1383–1399.

Pope, D. R., Edwards, M., & Schor, C. S. (1999b). Extraction of depth from opposite-contrast stimuli: Transient system can, sustained system can’t. Vision Research, 39(24), 4010–4017.

Riesenhuber, M., Jarudi, I., Gilad, S., & SInha, P. (2004). Face processing in humans is compatible with a simple shape-based model of vision. Proceedings of Royal Society of London B, 271(Suppl. 6), S448–S450.

Riesenhuber, M., & Poggio, T. (1999). Hierarchical models of object recognition in cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 1019–1025.

Rock, I., Gopnik, A., & Hall, S. (1994). Do young-children reverse ambiguous figures.  Perception, 23(6), 635–644

Sato, T. (1983). Depth seen with subjective contours. Japanese Psychological Research, 25(4), 213–221.

Schor, C. M., Edwards, M., & Pope, D. R. (1998). Spatial-frequency and contrast tuning of the transient-stereopsis system. Vision Research,38(20), 3057–3068.

Schor, C. M., Edwards, M., & Sato, M. (2001). Envelope size tuning for stereo-depth perception of small and large disparities. Vision Research,41(20), 2555–2567.

Sergent, J. (1984a). An investigation into component and configurational processes underlying face perception. British Journal of Psychology, 75, 221–242.

Sergent, J. (1984b). Configural processing of faces in the left and right cerebral hemispheres. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 10, 554–572.

Sheliga, B. M., FitzGibbon, E. J., & Miles, F. A. (2006). Short-latency disparity vergence eye movements: A response to disparity energy. Vision Research, 46, 3723–3740.

Sheliga, B. M., FitzGibbon, E. J., & Miles, F. A. (2007). Human vergence eye movements initiated by competing disparities: Evidence for a winner-take-all mechanism. Vision Research, 47, 479–500.

Takemura, A., Murata, Y., Kawano, K., & Miles, F. A. (2007). Deficits in short-latency tracking eye movements after chemical lesions in monkey cortical areas MT and MSTJournal of Neuroscience, 27, 529–541.

Tanaka, J., & Farah, M. (1993). Parts and wholes in face recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46A, 225–245.

Tanaka, J., & Sengco, J. (1997). Features and their configuration in face recognition. Memory and Cognition, 25, 583–592.

Tarr, M. J., & Pinker, S. (1989). Mental rotation and orientation dependence in shape recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 233–282.

Tittle J S, Todd J T, Perotti V J, Norman J F, 1995 The systematic distortion of perceived 3D structure from motion and binocular stereopsis, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 663-678.

Tse, P. A. & Logothetis, N. K. (2002) The duration of 3-D form analysis in transformational apparent motion. Perception & Psychophysics 64, 244-265.

Ullman S, 1978 Two-dimensionality of the correspondence process in apparent motion'' Perception 7, 683-689.

Vishwanath D., Girshick A. R. & Banks M. S. 2005  Why pictures look right when viewed from the wrong place. Nature Neuroscience 8, 1401-1410.

Watanabe, K., Paik, Y., & Blake, R. (2004). Preserved gain control for luminance contrast during binocular rivalry suppression. Vision Research, 44, 3065–3071.

Weyrauch, B., Huang, J., Heisele, B., & Blanz, V. (2003). Component-based face recognition with 3D morphable models. Lecture Notes In Computer Science, 2688, 27–34.

Wilcox, L. M., & Hess, R. F. (1995). Dmax for stereopsis depends on size, not spatial frequency content. Vision Research, 35(8),1061–1069.

Wilcox, L. M., & Hess, R. F. (1997). Scale selection for second-order (nonlinear) stereopsis. Vision Research, 37(21), 2981–2992.

Wilcox, L. M., & Hess, R. F. (1998). When stereopsis does not improve with increasing contrast. Vision Research, 38(23), 3671–3679.

Wilson, H. R. (1977). Hysteresis in binocular grating perception: Contrast effects. Vision Research, 17(7), 843–851.

Yang T. & Kubovy M. 1999 Weakening the robustness of perspective: Evidence for a modified theory of compensation in picture perception. Perception & Psychophysics 61, 456-467.

Young M J, Landy M S, Maloney L T, (1993) A perturbation analysis of depth perception from combinations of texture and motion cues. Vision Research 33, 2685-2696.

Zhao, W., Chellappa, R., Phillips, P., & Rosenfeld, A. (2003). Face recognition: A literature survey. ACM Computing Surveys, 35, 399–458.