参考文献

Andersen, R.A.(1997).Neural mechanisms of visual motion perception in primates.

    Neuron, 18, 865872.

Arnold, D. H. (2011). Why is binocular rivalry uncommon? Discrepant monocular

  images in the real world. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 116.

Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., Ashwin, C., Tavassoli, T., & Chakrabarti, B. (2009).

    Talent in autism: Hyper-systemizing, hyper-attention to detail and sensory 

    hypersensitivity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London.

    Series B, Biological sciences, 364(1522), 13771383.

Backus, B. T., & Haijiang, Q. (2007). Competition between newly recruited and  

    preexisting visual cues during the construction of visual appearance. Vision

    Research, 47(7), 919924.

Bartels, A., & Logothetis, N. (2010). Binocular rivalry: A time dependence of eye

    and stimulus contributions. Journal of Vision, 10, 114.

Bi, T., Cai, P., Zhou, T., & Fang, F. (2009). The effect of crowding on orientation

    selective adaptation in human early visual cortex. Journal of Vision, 9(11),

    13,1110.

Bidwell,L. C., Holzman, P. S., & Chen, Y. (2006). Aging and visual motion

    discrimination in normal adults and schizophrenia patients. Psychiatry Research,

    145, 18.

Bingham, G. P., & Lind, M. (2008). Large continuous perspective transformations are

  necessary and sufficient for accurate perception of metric shape. Perception &

  Psychophysics, 70(3), 524540.

Blake, R., Yu, K., Lokey, M., & Norman, H. (1998) Binocular rivalry and motion 

    perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(1), 4660.

Blake, R., Tadin, D., Sobel, K. V., Raissian, T. A., & Chong, S. C. (2006). Strength

    of early visual adaptation depends on visual awareness. Proceedings of the

    National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(12), 4783-

 4788.

Bradshaw, M. F., Hibbard, P. B., Parton, A. D., Rose,D., & Langley, K. (2006). Surface

 orientation,modulation frequency and the detection and perception of depth

 defined by binocular disparity and motion parallax. Vision Research, 46(17),2636


 2644.

Bradshaw, M. F., & Rogers, B. J. (1999). Sensitivity to horizontal and vertical

    corrugations defined by binocular disparity. Vision Research, 39(18), 30493056.

Chakravarthi, R., & Cavanagh, P. (2009). Recovery of a crowded object by masking

 the flankers: Determining the locus of feature integration. Journal of Vision,

  9(10),4, 1
9.

Chong, S. C., Tadin, D., & Blake, R. (2005). Endogenous attention prolongs dominance


  durations in binocular rivalry. Journal of Vision, 5(11), 10041012.

Chung, S. T., Levi, D. M., & Legge, G. E. (2001). Spatial-frequency and contrast 

    properties of crowding. Vision Research, 41(14), 18331850.

Ciner, E. B., Scheiman, M. M., Schanel-Klitsch, E., & Weil, L. (1989). Stereopsis

  testing in 18- to 35-month old children using operant preferential looking.

  Optometry & Vision Science, 66, 782
787.

Cooper, J., Feldman, J., & Medlin, D. (1979). Comparing stereoscopic performance of

    children using the Titmus, TNO, and Randot stereo tests. Journal of the

  American
Optometric Association, 50, 821825.

Doerschner, K., Fleming, R., Yilmaz, O., Schrater, P.,Hartung, B., & Kersten, D.  

  (2011). Visual motion and the perception of surface material. Current Biology,  

     21, 20102016.

Durgin, F. H., & Li, Z. (2011). Perceptual scale expansion: An efficient angular

  codingstrategy for locomotor space. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics,

  73(6), 1856
1870.

Edden, R. A. E., Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., Freeman, T. C. A., & Singh, K. D. (2009).

    Orientation discrimination performance is predicted by GABA concentration and 

    gamma oscillation frequency in human primary visual cortex. Journal of 

    Neuroscience, 29, 1572115726.

Fox, R., Patterson, R., & Francis, E. (1986). Stereoacuity in young children. 

    Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 27, 598600.

Freeman, A. W. (2005). Multistage model for binocular rivalry. Journal of  

  Neurophysiology, 94(6), 44124420.

Freeman, J., Donner, T. H., & Heeger, D. J. (2011). Inter-area correlations in the  

    ventral visual pathway reflect feature integration. Journal of Vision, 11(4),

    123.
Greenwood, J. A., Bex, P. J., & Dakin, S. C. (2012). Crowding follows the binding of

  relative position and orientation. Journal of Vision, 12(3), 18, 1
20.

Gwiazda,J.,Bauer,J. & Held,R. 1989 Binocular function in human infants : Correlation

    of stereoscopic and fusion-rivalry discriminations Journal of Pediatric  

    Ophthalmology & Strabismus,26,128-132.

Haijiang, Q., Saunders, J. A., Stone, R. W., & Backus, B. T. (2006). Demonstration of

  cue recruitment: Change in visual appearance by means of Pavlovian

  conditioning.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United

  States of America,
103, 483488.

Hancock, S., & Andrews, T. J. (2007). The role of voluntary and involuntary attention

    in selecting perceptual dominance during binocular rivalry. Perception, 36,   

    288298.

Harrison, S. J., & Backus, B. T. (2012). Associative learning of shape as a cue to 

  appearance: A new demonstration of cue recruitment. Journal of Vision, 12(3),

  1-17.


Heravian-Shandiz, J., Douthwaite, W. A., & Jenkins,T. C. (1991). Binocular

 interaction with neutral density filters as measured by the visual evoked

  response. Optometry & Vision Science, 68(10), 801
806.

Heron, G., Dholakia, S., Collins, D. E., & McLaughlan, H. (1985). Stereoscopic

    threshold in children and adults. American Journal of Optometry & Physiological

    Optics, 62, 505515

Hess, R.F.,Hutchinson,C.V.,Ledgeway,T.,&Mansouri,B.(2007). Binocular influences on

    global motion processing in the human visual system. Vision Research, 47, 1682-  

    1692.

Khuu,S.K. (2012) The role of motion streaks in the perception of the kinetic Zollner

    illusion. Journal of Vision, 12(6):19,1-14.

Katsumi, O., Tanino, T., & Hirose, T. (1986). Objective evaluation of binocular
 
  function using the pattern reversal visual evoked response. II. Effect of mean

  luminosity. Acta Ophthalmologica (Copenhagen), 64(2), 199
205.

Knapen, T., Paffer, C., Kanai, R., & van Ee, R. (2007). Stimulus flicker alters

  interocular grouping during binocular rivalry. Vision Research, 47, 1
7.

Kovács, I., Papathomas, T. V., Yang, M., & Fehér, Á. (1996). When the brain changes

    its mind: Interocular grouping during binocular rivalry. Proceedings of the

    National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93, 1550815511.

Leat, S. J., St Pierre, J., Hasan-Abadi, S., & Faubert, J. (2001). The moving dynamic

    random dot stereosize test: Development, age norms, and comparison with the

    Frisby, Randot and Stereo Smile tests. Journal of Pediatric Ophthalmology &

    Strabismus, 38, 284294.

Leekam, S. R., Nieto, C., Libby, S. J., Wing, L., & Gould, J. (2007). Describing the

    sensory abnormalities of children and adults with autism Journal of Autism and 

    Developmental Disorders, 37(5), 894910.

Lehky, S. R. (1988). An astable multivibrator model of binocular rivalry.

  Perception,
17(2), 215
228.

Leventhal, A. G., Wang, Y., Pu, M., Zhou, Y., & Ma, Y. (2003). GABA and its agonists

    improved visual cortical function in senescent monkeys. Science, 300, 812815.

Levi, D. M. (2008). Crowding An essential bottleneck for object recognition: A     

    mini-review. Vision Research, 48(5), 635654.

Li, Z., & Durgin, F. H. (2009). Downhill slopes look shallower from the edge. Journal

  of Vision, 9(11):6,1
15.

Li, Z., & Durgin, F. H. (2012). A comparison of two theories of perceived distance on

  the ground plane:The angular expansion hypothesis and the intrinsic bias

  hypothesis. i-Perception, 3(5), 368.


Li, Z., Phillips, J., & Durgin, F. H. (2011). The underestimation of egocentric distance:

    Evidence from frontal matching tasks. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics,

    73(7), 22052217.

Li, Z., Sun, E., Strawser, C. J., Spiegel, A., Klein, B., & Durgin, F. H. (2013). On the 

 anisotropy of perceived ground extents and the interpretation of walked distance

 as a measure of perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human

  Perception and Performance, 39(2), 477.


Li, J., Thompson, B., Ding, Z., Chan, L. Y., Chen, X.,Yu, M., et al. (2012). Does partial

 occlusion promote normal binocular function? Investigative Ophthalmology &

 Visual Science, 53(11), 6818
6827.

Liang, Z., Yang, Y., Li, G., Zhang, J., Wang, Y., Zhou, Y., et al. (2010). Aging affects

 the direction selectivity of MT cells in rhesus monkeys. Neurobiology of Aging,

 31(5),863
873.

Machilsen, B., & Wagemans, J. (2011). Integration of contour and surface information

 in shape detection. Vision Research, 51, 179
186.

Meng, M., & Tong, F. (2004) Can attention selectively bias bistable perception?     

    Differences between binocular rivalry and ambiguous figures. Journal of Vision,4

    539551.

Noest, A. J., van Ee, R., Nijs, M. M., & van Wezel, R.J. (2007). Percept-choice

 sequences driven by interrupted ambiguous stimuli: A low-level neural model.

 Journal of Vision, 7(8):10, 1
14.

Norman,J. F., Ross,H.E., Hawkes,L.M., & Long,J.R. (2003). Aging and the perception  

   of speed. Perception, 32, 8596.

Ooi, T. L., & He, Z. J. (1999). Binocular rivalry and visual awareness: The role of 

    attention. Perception, 28(5), 551574.

OShea, R. P. (2011). Binocular rivalry stimuli are common but rivalry is not.

  Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 148.

Otero-Millan, J.,Troncoso,X.G.,Macknik,S.L.,Serrano-Pedraza,I. & Martinez-Conde,S.  

    (2008). Saccades and microsaccades during visual fixation, exploration, and

    search: Foundations for a common saccadic generator. Journal of  Vision, 8, 118.

Parkes, L., Lund, J., Angelucci, A., Solomon, J. A., & Morgan, M. (2001). Compulsory

    averaging of crowded orientation signals in human vision. Nature Neuroscience,4, 

    739744.

Pelli, D. G., & Tillman, K. A. (2008). The uncrowded window of object recognition.

  Nature Neuroscience, 11(10), 11291135.

Raghuram, A., Lakshminarayanan, V., & Khanna, R. (2005). Psychophysical

 estimation  
of speed discrimination. II. Aging effects. Journal of the Optical

 Society of
America A, 22, 22692280.

Romano, P. E., Romano, J. A., & Puklin, J. E. (1975). Stereoacuity development in

    children with normal binocular single vision. American Journal of

    Ophthalmology, 79(6), 966971.

Rubenstein, J. L., & Merzenich, M. M. (2003). Model of autism: Increased ratio of

    excitation/inhibition in key neural systems. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 2(5),   

    255267.

Sassi, M., Vancleef, K., Machilsen, B., Panis, S., & Wagemans, J. (2010).

    Identification of everyday objects on the basis of Gaborized outline versions.

    i-Perception, 1, 121142.

Serrano-Pedraza, I., & Read, J. C. A. (2010). Multiple channels for horizontal, but

    only one for vertical corrugations? A new look at the stereo anisotropy. Journal

    of Vision, 10(12):10, 111.

Simons, K. (1981). Stereoacuity norms in young children. Archives of Ophthalmology,

    99, 439445.

Simons, K. (1993). Stereoscopic neurontropy and the origins of amblyopia and

    strabismus. In K. Simons (Ed.), Early visual development: Normal and abnormal

    (pp. 409453). New York: Oxford University Press.

Snowden, R. J., & Kavanagh, E. (2006). Motion perception in the ageing visual

 system:
Minimum motion, motion coherence, and speed discrimination thresholds. 

 Perception, 35, 924.

Tomac, S., & Altay, Y. (2000). Near stereoacuity: Development in preschool children;

    normative values and screening for binocular vision abnormalities; a study of

    115 children. Binocular Vision Strabismus Quarterly, 15, 221228.

Tomchek, S. D., & Dunn, W. (2007). Sensory processing in children with and without  

    autism: A comparative study using the short sensory profile. American Journal

    of Occupational Therapy, 61(2), 190200.

van der Willigen, R. F., Harmening, W. M., Vossen, S.,& Wagner, H. (2010). Disparity

    sensitivity in man and owl: Psychophysical evidence for equivalent perception

    of shape-from-stereo. Journal of Vision, 10(1):10, 111.

van Ee, R., van Dam, L. C. J., & Brouwer, G. J. (2005). Voluntary control and the 

    dynamics of perceptual bi-stability. Vision Research, 45, 4155.

Vattikuti, S., & Chow, C. C. (2010). A computational model for cerebral cortical 

    dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 67(7), 672678.

Whitney, D., & Levi, D. M. (2011). Visual crowding: A fundamental limit on

 conscious
perception and object recognition. Trends in Cognitive Science,

 15(4),160
168.

Wilcox, L. M., & Hess, R. (1995). Dmax for stereopsis depends on size not spatial

    frequency content. Vision Research, 35, 10611069.

Wilcox, L. M., & Hess, R. F. (1996). Is the site of non-linear filtering in stereopsis

    before or after binocular combination? Vision Research, 36, 391399.

Wilcox, L. M., & Hess, R. F. (1997). Scale selection for second-order (non-linear

    stereopsis. Vision Research, 37, 29812992.

Wilcox, L. M., & Hess, R. (1998). When stereopsis does not improve with increasing

    contrast. Vision Research, 38, 36713679.

Wilson, H. R. (2003). Computational evidence for a rivalry hierarchy in vision.

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 100(24),1449914503.

Zhang, P., Bobier, W., Thompson, B., & Hess, R. F.(2011). Binocular balance in normal

    vision and its modulation by mean luminance. Optometry & Vision Science, 88(9),

    10721079.